When a song is on your lips, Bates wants to know what’s in your brain

Janet Bourne, Mellon Postdoctoral Fellow in the Humanities, teaches Music Theory II in the Olin Arts Center on March 30. (Phyllis Graber Jensen/Bates College)

Janet Bourne, Mellon Postdoctoral Fellow in the Humanities, teaches Music Theory II in the Olin Arts Center on March 30, 2016. Bourne was recruited to help integrate music cognition into the music theory curriculum. (Phyllis Graber Jensen/Bates College)

Why is it that many of us can recognize the Spice Girls’ “Wannabe” or Roy Orbison’s “Oh, Pretty Woman” from the very first notes?

Why doesn’t country star Mel Tillis’ speech impediment affect his singing?

Why do we hear some sounds as music and others as noise? Why do we disagree about which is which? And how exactly does music make us feel happy or sad?

Such questions are the province of music cognition, a field of study that harnesses musicology, psychology, neuroscience, and other disciplines to understand music’s workings in the brain. The field has blossomed in the last 20 or 25 years, says Bill Matthews, Alice Swanson Esty Professor of Music — even as cognitive science, in general, has been redefined by sophisticated neurological scanning capabilities and other technological advances.

William Matthews, Alice Swanson Esty Professor of Music at Bates, is developing a music cognition textbook. (Doug Hubley/Bates College)

William Matthews, Alice Swanson Esty Professor of Music at Bates, is developing a music cognition textbook. (Doug Hubley/Bates College)

Bates is humming along with this fascinating field. Matthews has introduced the topic into the music theory curriculum over the last several years, even developing a textbook. Associate Professor Gina Fatone’s research lies in the intersection of cognitive science and musical experience.

Their colleague James Parakilas, James L. Moody, Jr. Family Professor of Performing Arts, designed a course called “Music and Mind” that has become one of the music department’s most popular offerings.

And now Bates is taking steps to formally integrate music cognition into the four-course theory curriculum, thanks to a pedagogy innovation program funded by the Andrew W. Mellon Foundation. The college last year recruited a music cognition expert to help with that process: Janet Bourne, one of Bates’ Mellon Postdoctoral Fellows in the Humanities.

As evidenced by the success of Parakilas’ course, cognition in the musical context has a strong appeal to students, especially those focusing on psychology and neuroscience, as well as music. “It’s one of those things that can be personally compelling and then it turns into an academic interest,” says Fatone, the department chair. “Music is important to everybody on a very, very personal level.”

“I wonder all the time while I’m playing how music works and what’s going on in my brain,” says trumpeter and pianist Grace Link ’19 of Montclair, N.J. Eager to study both music and neuroscience, she is taking Music Theory II this semester with Bourne, who has introduced a solid dose of music cognition into the course.

Assigned to adapt a pop song to an 18th-century idiom, Will McPherson ’18 chose the Beatles’ “Girl” (Lennon-McCartney).


“I find muscle memory and learning a piece of music so fascinating,” Link says — “how you can learn something complicated and be able to play it back. And also how our brain receives different types of music, and the interactions and the emotions it triggers. I think it’s all really cool.”

Bourne, who earned a doctorate in music theory and cognition from Northwestern last year, has an active role in building music cognition into the curriculum. She’s collaborating with music faculty to create learning objectives and assessments, as well as an evaluation plan for the curriculum. She’s also bringing in top music theory and music psychology scholars to advise the department on the process.

“Bates is one of the only places I know of that’s purposely trying to infuse music cognition into its music theory curriculum.”

“We chose Janet,” says Parakilas, “because she comes out of a program at Northwestern that’s one of the leading music theory programs with that cognitive perspective” — as well as one of the very few programs in the field. “Her work impressed us.”

For the Music Theory II course, Fergus Scott ’17 adapted Carly Rae Jepsen’s “Call Me Maybe” (Jepsen-Ramsay-Crowe) to an 18th-century keyboard style.


Bourne is one of the relatively rare scholars who approach music cognition from the discipline of music, rather than psychology or neuroscience — which makes her a good fit for Bates because, as she says, “Bates is one of the only places I know of that’s purposely trying to infuse music cognition into its music theory curriculum.”

In her own research, Bourne focuses on patterns in music, how those patterns constitute musical conventions (think of the guitar introductions to “Oh, Pretty Woman,” the Beatles’ “Day Tripper,” and the Temptations’ “My Girl”), how those conventions relate to conventional patterns in language — and how our minds categorize both types of convention.

Students sing in an improvisation exercise in a Music Theory II class on March 30, 2016. (Phyllis Graber Jensen/Bates College)

Students sing in an improvisation exercise in a Music Theory II class on March 30, 2016. (Phyllis Graber Jensen/Bates College)

She also looks at the role of analogy, or the functioning of shared relationships, in our comprehension of music. “There are some cognitive psychologists who would say that analogy is at the core of everything that we do,” Bourne says. But analogy as an element of music cognition hasn’t been deeply explored.

In a recent paper, Bourne argues that “listeners perceive irony in music by making an analogy between music and language — in other words, passages of music that are analogous to passages of ironic language would be perceived as ironic.”

One form of irony in language is to change the meaning of a concept by changing the context in which it’s used. The same applies in music: Imagine a blues song in which the singer’s Christmas is spoiled because his lover left him, and then he quotes “Jingle Bells” in his guitar solo. That’s a lonely one-horse sleigh.

At Bates, Music Theory II typically looks at conventions of the 18th century — such as chord progressions, counterpoint, and improvisational tactics — many of which continue to influence today’s music vocabulary. (For one class assignment, students adapted modern pop songs such as the Beatles’ “Girl” or Carly Rae Jepson’s “Call Me Maybe” to an 18th-century keyboard idiom.)

“Our department has always been very forward-looking.”

But, through readings and class projects, Bourne is also explicating not only the musical vocabulary of Mozart’s time, but relevant cognitive practices. “I’m trying to connect this to the reasons that some 18th-century practices are still used,” she says.

Take cadences, those musical gestures that signal the end of a phrase. Bourne’s students heard music theorists’ explanations for the differing experiences of finality produced by different cadences.

But she also had the class read about an experiment that tested how listeners, both musically trained and not, perceived various cadences. “The experiment shows that you don’t need musical training to hear most of the cadences as closing gestures,” Bourne explains.

Students in Music Theory II work with Janet Bourne in an improvisational singing exercise. (Phyllis Graber Jensen/Bates College)

Janet Bourne, at right, is one of the rare scholars who approach music cognition from the discipline of music, rather than psychology or neuroscience. She is shown with students in a Music Theory II class; from left, Justin Demers ’18 of Nottingham, N.H., Will McPherson ’18 of Berwyn, Penn., and Thai Tran ’19 of Hanoi, Vietnam. (Phyllis Graber Jensen/Bates College)

Popularity with students is just one of the arguments supporting a systemic role for cognition in Bates’ music theory offering. More objectively, “it makes a better-rounded theory structure,” says Maddie McLean ’17, a double major in biochemistry and music from Hood River, Ore.

“In high school, you’d learn theory and be like, ‘Well, why does that work?’ And the answer was, ‘Because it does.’ And that’s not a great answer.” Studying music from psychological or cognitive angles, she says, “just gives more context.”

Music cognition’s power to connect with other fields is obvious. In last fall’s iteration of “Music and Mind,” Jim Parakilas and his students roamed a range of topics that extended from music therapy to whale song to music and infant development.

Another benefit, says Bill Matthews, pertains — in a keenly liberal-arts manner — to student intellectual development. “This has to do with what’s called metacognition,” he says.

“If you have insight into your own learning processes, if you are constantly aware of how you are learning and how you are processing information, I think it enhances learning —it makes it more efficient, and we can learn more when we’re aware of that.”

Ian Clarkson set The Temptations’ “Papa Was a Rolling Stone” (Whitfield-Strong) to an 18th-century idiom.


Bourne’s work with the music theory program, her music department colleagues hope, could be the beginning of a broader infusion of psychology into the department’s curriculum.

“Everyone in the department is interested in this field in their own way,” says Parakilas. “Once you start thinking about this, you think, ‘Well, what about our instrumental instruction?’ ‘What about our musicology courses?’ ‘What about ethnomusicology?'”

“Our department has always been very forward-looking,” says Fatone. “This is an exciting new turn for music to take.”

Categories Arts